Journal of Clinical Images and Medical Case Reports

ISSN 2766-7820
Research Article - Open Access, Volume 5

A study of improvement in language dysfunction among patients with psychosis from a tertiary care centre from North India

Aditi Jain*; Abbas Mehdi

Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Career Institute of Medical Sciences and Hospital, Lucknow, Uttar Pradesh, India.

*Corresponding Author : Aditi Jain
Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry, Career Institute of Medical Sciences and Hospital, Lucknow, Uttar Pradesh, India.
Email: [email protected]

Received : Nov 26, 2024

Accepted : Dec 13, 2024

Published : Dec 20, 2024

Archived : www.jcimcr.org

Copyright : © Jain A (2024).

Abstract

Background: Language dysfunction is an objective clinical marker of brain dysfunction in psychosis which encompasses conditions like schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and bipolar affective disorder with psychotic symptoms.

Aim: To study the improvement in language dysfunction with treatment among patients of psychosis over a period of 12 weeks using the Clinical Language Disorder Rating Scale (CLANG).

Methodology: 60 patients with psychosis who attended the inpatient services of psychiatry department of a tertiary care center from North India in the period from January 2022 to October 2023 were evaluated for the presence of language dysfunction using a valid tool called CLANG at baseline, 4 weeks and 12 weeks after taking approval from the Institutional Ethical Committee and with the informed consent of the patients and/or their caregivers. Data set for 60 patients regarding the sociodemographic-clinical profiles and language dysfunction were analysed using descriptive and inferential statistics as appropriate.

Results: mild, moderate and severe dysfunction were combined, all patients were found to have some language dysfunction. The CLANG domains most affected in our patients at baseline were referential failures (65%), discourse failures (46.67%), poverty of speech (38.33%), lack of details (33.33%), abnormal prosody (28.33%), aprosodic speech (21.67%), excess details (21.67%) and lack of semantic association (18.33%). The factors which were maximally improved over 12 weeks were referential failures (50%), discourse failures (30%), poverty of speech (25%), lack of details (21.67%) and abnormal prosody (18.33%).

Conclusion: It is evident from our study that language dysfunction is an important component of psychosis. Hence our study revalidates the neural basis of psychosis.

Keywords: Language; Psychosis; Schizophrenia; Clinical language disorder rating scale; North India.

Citation: Jain A, Mehdi A. A study of improvement in language dysfunction among patients with psychosis from a tertiary care centre from North India. J Clin Images Med Case Rep. 2024; 5(12): 3397.

Introduction

Language is a fundamental experience of human experience that undergoes profound disruption in psychotic disorders like schizophrenia and bipolar affective disorder with psychotic symptoms. According to researchers, Schizophrenia is the price that “Homo sapiens” have to pay for language [1].

The earliest attempt to study the abnormal nature of verbal impairments in schizophrenia. Was made in the early 20th century by [2]. Almost four decades later in 1979, Nancy Andreasen found out that several aspects of language were abnormal in patients with schizophrenia, viz. comprehension, attentional shifts in the sentences, pragmatics, semantic organization, referential failures, paucity of speech and fluency [3,4].

According to researchers, language impairment is one of the core phenomenological characteristics of patients with schizophrenia and it has been proposed that there must be some deficits in the neural organization of language in schizophrenic patients [5].

Compared to classical psychotic symptoms (such as delusions and hallucinations), language dysfunction can be directly observed and it is not dependent upon the subjective report of abnormal experiences on part of the patient which is in contrast to delusions and hallucinations the evaluation of which relies heavily on subjective report by the patients. Consequently, language dysfunction is now being regarded as a more objective and informative clinical marker of brain dysfunction in psychosis [3-5].

Various international studies that have examined speech samples of schizophrenia patients, have reported problems at multiple levels of language processing i.e. lexical, syntactic, semantic and discourse levels [6-11].

Need for the study

As reported by previous studies, there is a dearth of Indian studies in the domain of language dysfunction in psychosis [12].

In a review of Indian studies in the domain of language and schizophrenia by Sultan et al. it was concluded that the number of studies done in India is very meagre and when compared to international studies, there is a strong need for revival of research in this area [13]. Furthermore, there are virtually no studies that have attempted to study the course and progress of language dysfunction in psychosis.

Through the current study, we envisage to take a step further toward better understanding of the multifaceted nature of language dysfunction in psychosis and also to evaluate their improvement at follow ups.

Aim: To evaluate the language dysfunction among patients of psychosis

Objectives

To examine the presence of disintegration of the components of language in untreated cases of psychosis using Clinical Language disorder rating scale (CLANG) [14] at baseline, 4 weeks and 12 weeks.

To see the improvement in CLANG domains among our patients with treatment over 12 weeks follow up period.

Materials and methods

Setting: Department of Psychiatry at a tertiary care centre from North India.

Study design: A short- term prospective study.

Approval of the institutional ethics committee for our study was taken in the month of December 2021

Study duration

The duration for recruitment of patients from inpatient services-January 2022 to October 2023 (almost 22 months)

The duration for short term follow-up from baseline-12 weeks (which was completed till January 2024.

Phases of assessment of patients

First assessment: Baseline.

Second assessment: At 4 weeks from baseline.

Final assessment: At 12 weeks from baseline.

Protocol for recruitment and phases of our study

In the period from 1st January 2022 to 30th October 2023, there were a total of 303 admissions to the psychiatric ward under a broad diagnosis of psychosis including schizophrenia. Out of these 303, 88 patients were those patients who had never received any prior treatment before coming to us.

By purposive sampling, we chose 60 patients for our study (those patients were selected who had good findings after fulfilling the inclusion and exclusion criteria).

Sample size: A total of 60 patients of psychosis.

Inclusion criteria

• Males and females

• Age 18 to 60 years

• Psychosis diagnosed using ICD 10 criteria [15].

• Patients who had never received any psychiatric treatment prior to admission to our department.

Exclusion criteria

• Subnormal intelligence

• Presence of comorbid substance use disorder

• Non psychotic mood disorder

• History of learning disabilities

• History of expressive speech disorder

• Hearing impairment

• Stress related speech disorder like emotional numbness in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)

• Presence of catatonic symptoms such as mutism and stupor

• Aphasia/dysphasia secondary to laryngeal or cerebral dysfunction

• Those who refused to be a part of our study (for any reason).

Description of CLANG in brief

It is a simple rating instrument which is based on modern psycholinguistic framework. This scale was validated in an extensive sample of 204 Hong Kong Chinese schizophrenic patients. It consists of 17 observer rated items anchored on a four-point severity scale, i.e., 0, 1, 2 and 3 (0 = Normal, 1 = Mild, no more than 10% of the time, 2 = Moderate, regular occurrence 10 to 50% of the time, 3 = Severe, pervasive, more than 50% of the time) Rating is based on verbal output during a period of conversation with the patient (lasting at least 15 minutes). Factor analysis done by Chen et al. (1966) revealed three major domains of language disorder captured by the scale: the semantic level, the syntactic level and the production level. The internal consistency of the CLANG and the relative contribution of individual items as found out by applying Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, proved that the internal reliability of the subscales is high (alpha coefficient for semantics subscale 0.76), for syntax subscale 0.80, and for production subscale 0.72. The intra-class correlation coefficient for the syntax subscale is 0.93, for the semantics subscale is 0.83 and for the production subscale is 0.88. Thus, CLANG is a reliable, valid and informative instrument for the clinical assessment of language disorder in schizophrenia [14].

Description of 17 items of the CLANG scale

Excess phonetic association: Abnormal association based on phonetic similarity (punning and clang associations).

Abnormal syntactic structure: Violations of ordinary rules of grammar leading to incomprehensible speech.

Excessive syntactic constraints: Excessive application of rigid grammatical structure to speech output, producing language that is “formal” and lack of flexibility of ordinary spoken language.

Lack of semantic association: Lack of normal semantic association between ideas expressed successively

Referential failures: Unclear links which leave excessive ambiguity as to which expressions refers back (or forth) to which items in preceding and subsequent speech.

Discourse failures (loss of schematic organization): Lack of the normal organization in which speech units, (eg. One or two sentences or above) progresses from one context to the other in a gradual and prepared manner.

Excessive details: Details given grossly in excess of that required in the given context.

Lack of details: Details given (though judged to be probably appropriate in meaning) grossly inadequate to context.

Aprosodic speech: Flat monotonous speech without appropriate inflection and emotional quality.

Abnormal prosody: Bizarre quality of voice, eg., high pitch, mechanical etc.

Pragmatic disorder: Speech content reflects defective knowledge of the world (judged to be independent of delusiondelusional ideas, i.e., of personal significance etc).

Dysfluency: Stuttering, false starts, hesitations.

Dysarthria: Articulation difficulties. Poverty of speech Reduced overall speech output.

Pressure of speech: Increased speech of word production as if a rapid internal production process paces speech.

Neologisms: Construction of idiosyncratic new words for personal use.

Paraphasic error: Substitution of word by words with similar meaning (but inappropriate and less precise).

Ethical considerations

Before starting the study, approval for this study was obtained from the Institutional Ethics Committee.

Written informed consent was obtained from the patients and /or their relatives after asking them to go through the patient information sheet printed in the local languages commonly used (Hindi) and a verbal explanation by the interviewer

The nature and purpose of the study was explained to them and also the need to cooperate for follow ups at least at 4 weeks and at 12 weeks and to provide us their contact address and phone numbers.

Confidentiality of the information provided was maintained.

No beneficial treatment was withheld and treatment was not altered in any way to facilitate intake into the study.

How data collection was done

Starting from January 2022, we aimed at enrolling patients fulfilling the inclusion and exclusion criteria for our study and his/her diagnosis based on ICD 10 was confirmed by consultant psychiatrist of the level of professor. Diagnosis of schizophrenia was not essential for inclusion into the study, rather expression of verbal or written speech were given more importance in our patients of untreated psychosis. Thus, out of 60 patients, we had 26 patients with diagnosis of schizophrenia rest of them were having diagnoses other than schizophrenia as mentioned vide infra in (Table 1) of our observations. For each patient, we took informed consent in the local language of the patient. The sociodemographic profile sheet was filled up as per the information given by the patient as well as at least one reliable informant staying with the patient for most part of his/her life. The socio demographic profile sheet covered parameters like name, age, sex, marital status, mother tongue, education, occupation, duration of untreated psychosis, diagnosis, age of onset and family history of psychiatric illness and treatment. For the purpose of establishing psychiatric diagnoses, we used ICD 10. All the 60 patients were then subjected to the administration of CLANG scale for the assessment of their language. For the purpose of eliciting a sufficient speech sample, we engaged the patient in conversation for at least 15 minutes under a standardized condition. The patient was asked to speak spontaneously for at least 3 to 5 minutes or write a paragraph on any of the following topics of their preference like my family, festivals of India, education system, status of India Pakistan relationship or any other topic of their choice. We kept in mind that we subjected the patients to open-ended questions rather than closed-ended ones which helped us elicit a sufficient speech sample in order to be able to apply the scale efficiently for language assessment. In some cases, we even showed the patients pictures and asked them to speak on it. The speech samples of the patients were audiotaped as well as video recorded. Later on, they were meticulously scrutinized for the presence of language dysfunction as defined in the CLANG scale.

They were then given appointments and their contact address and phone numbers were noted. A day prior to their respective appointment, the patient’s family was cordially reminded telephonically to ensure their scheduled follow up (first follow up at week 4 and second follow up at week 12 respectively). These were again evaluated for status of their language abnormalities at both the follow ups to see and note the degree of improvement in language abnormalities with treatment in this short prospective period of 12 weeks. The results /observations were noted down at each assessment for all 60 patients. Fortunately, we are able to evaluate all 60 patients without any attrition at week 4 and week 12 albeit with a delay of one or two days.

The results were noted down for all 60 patients.

Outcome parameters

1. The presence of disintegration of the components of language in diagnosed and untreated acute psychosis patients using CLANG.

2. The degree of improvement in language abnormalities with antipsychotics first at 4weeks and then at 12 weeks.

Statistical analysis

The data analysis was done using SPSS 20.0 version. In descriptive statistics, we used simple measures like frequency and percentage for ordinal and nominal variables for the sociodemographic and clinical profiles of the patients. For inferential statistics, we used one way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA). The p-value of 0.05 has been considered to be statistically significant and a p value of 0.005 to be highly significant.

Results

(Table 1) of our results shows the sociodemographic and clinical parameters like age, gender, occupation, education, marital status, age of onset of psychosis, family history, diagnosis, duration of untreated psychosis and psychopharmacological treatment administered to the patients in our study.

73.33% of our psychotic patients were in the age group of 21 to 30 years. The mean age was 25.4 years. The mean age of onset of psychosis in our patients was 23.9 years. Diagnosiswise, 43.33% of our patients had a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Patients with Schizoaffective disorder and acute and transient psychotic disorder were 11.67% each, 5% had persistent delusional disorder, 3.33% had other nonorganic psychotic disorder, 10% had severe depressive episodes with psychotic symptoms and 15% had bipolar affective disorder with psychotic symptoms

(Table 2) depicts the distribution of language dysfunction in our patients using CLANG Scale at baseline. When mild, moderate and severe language disturbances in the patients were clubbed together, the most commonly affected language domains were referential failures (65%). The second most common language disturbance in our study was discourse failures (46.67%). As regards prosody related language disturbances, abnormal prosody (28.33%) and aprosodic speech (21.67%) were seen in our patients. Abnormal syntax was seen in 16.67% of our patients. None of our patients had dysarthria or paraphasic error. Lack of semantic association was seen in 18.34% of our patients. Neologisms was seen in 6.67% of our patients

(Table 3) is a comprehensive reflection of language dysfunction among our patients from the point of entry to subsequent two follow ups. It shows the progressive improvement in each specific domain of CLANG in our patients from baseline to subsequent follow ups at week 4 and week 12. We found that all the CLANG domains showed improvement with treatment albeit to different extents. The factors that completely improved were excessive syntactic constraints, pragmatics disorder and neologisms. The improvement was more between 0 to 4 weeks for excessive phonetic association, abnormal syntax and excess syntactic constraints as well as lack of semantic association whereas referential failures, discourse failures, excess details and poverty of speech showed more improvement from week 4 to week 12.

Interestingly, neologisms, pragmatics disorder and excess syntactic constraints responded quite effectively and earlier compared to other domains.

(Table 4) shows the percentage improvement in CLANG domains in our patients over 12 weeks after their natural course of treatment. For the sake of understanding, we have only taken baseline and second follow up (at 12 weeks) i.e. between point of entry to point of exit, a span of 12 weeks which is considered a duration in our short term follow up study. This table depicts which language disturbance is improved and to what extent compared to which does not and we found that referential failures (50%) and discourse failures (30%) followed by poverty of speech (25%) and lack of details (21.67%) showed maximum level of improvement. The distribution was found to be significant for referential failures (p value 0.0001), discourse failures (p value 0.002), lack of details (p value 0.0001) and poverty of speech (p value 0.003).

(Graph 1) of our results depicts the CLANG factors maximally improved over 12 weeks with treatment.

Figure 1: CLANG factors maximally improved over 12 weeks in our patients
Table 1: The sociodemographic and clinical parameters like age, gender, occupation, education, marital status, age of onset of psychosis, family history, diagnosis, duration of untreated psychosis and psychopharmacological treatment being administered among the patients in our study.
Sociodemographic / clinical parameter No. of patientsn (%)
Age at time offirstadmission to hospital 21-30 years 44(73.33%)
31-40 years 13(21.7%)
41-50 years 3(5%)
Mean age at time of firstadmission to hospital 25.4years
Gender Male 38 (63.33%)
Female 22(36.67%)
Education Secondary 41 (68.3%)
Higher secondary 9 (15%)
Graduate 10(16.67%)
Occupation Unemployed 22(36.67%)
Semiskilled laborer 22 (36.67%)
Skilled laborer 8 (13.33%)
Clerk/farmer/shopkeeper 2(3.33%)
Semi-professional 6(10%)
Marital status Single 17(28.33%)
Married 30(50%)
Separated 9(15%)
divorced 4(6.67%)
Age of onset ofpsychosis 21-30 years 41(68.33%)
31-40 years 19(31.67%)
Mean age of onsetofpsychosis 23.9 years
Diagnosis Schizophrenia 26(43.33%)
Schizoaffective disorder 7(11.67%)
Persistent delusional disorder 3(5%)
Acute andtransientpsychotic disorders 7(11.67%)
Other nonorganicpsychotic disorders 2(3.33%)
Severe depressive episode with psychotic symptoms 6(10%)
Bipolar affective disorder with psychotic symptoms 9(15%)
Duration of untreatedpsychosis (in months) <12 5(8.33%)
12-24 40(66.67%)
24-36 9(15%)
36-48 6(10%)
Mean DUPin months 30±0.86
Psychopharmacological Treatment beingadministered Antipsychotic 38(63.33%)
Antipsychotic+MoodStabilizer+Benzodiazepines 7(11.67%)
Antidepressant+Benzodiazepines 6(10%)
Antidepressant+Mood Stabilizer+Benzodiazepines 9(15%)

Table 2: Distribution of language dysfunction in our patients using CLANG Scale at baseline.
CLANG domain Score as per CLANG No. of patients N (%) at baseline
Excess phonetic association Normal 52(86.67%)
Mild 4(6.67%)
Moderate 4(6.67%)
severe 0
Abnormal syntax Normal 50(83.33%)
Mild 5(8.33%)
Moderate 5(8.33%)
Severe 0
Excess syntactic constraints Normal 57(95%)
Mild 2(3.33%)
Moderate 1(1.67%)
Severe 0
Lack of semantic association Normal 49(81.67%)
Mild 3(5%)
Moderate 7(11.67%)
Severe 1(1.67%)
Referential failures Normal 21(35%)
Mild 12(20%)
Moderate 23(38.33%)
Severe 4(6.67%)
Discourse failure Normal 32(53.33%)
Mild 11(18.33%)
Moderate 15(25%)
Severe 2(3.33%)
Excess details Normal 47(78.33%)
Mild 5(8.33%)
Moderate 8(13.33%)
Severe 0
Lack of details Normals 40(66.67%)
Mild 10(16.67%)
Moderate 7 (11.67%)
Severe 3(5%)
Aprosodic speech Normal 47(78.33%)
Mild 8(13.33%)
Moderate 5(8.33%)
Severe 0
Abnormal prosody Normal 43(71.67%)
Mild 12(20%)
Moderate 5(8.33%)
Severe 0
Pragmatics disorder Normal 58(96.67%)
Mild 1(1.67%)
Moderate 1(1.67%)
Severe 0
Dysfluency Normal 52(86.67%)
Mild 5(8.33%)
Moderate 3(5%)
Severe 0
Dysarthria Normal 60(100%)
Mild 0
Moderate 0
Severe 0
Poverty of speech Normal 37(61.67%)
Mild 12(20%)
Moderate 11(18.33%)
Severe 0
Pressure of speech Normal 54(90%)
Mild 3(5%)
Moderate 2(3.33%)
Severe 1(1.67%)
Neologisms Normal 56(93.33%)
Mild 2(3.33%)
Moderate 2(3.33%)
Severe 0
Paraphasic error Normal 60(100%)
Mild 0
Moderate 0
Severe 0

Table 3: Progressive improvement in each domain(s) of CLANG in our patients from baseline to subsequent follow ups at week 4 and week 12.
CLANG domain Number of patients showinglanguage dysfunction (mild,moderate, severe combined)
Baseline N(out of 60) % 4 weeks N(out of 60) % 12 weeks N(out of 60) % F value Pvalue
Excess phonetic association 8 13.33 5 8.33 2 3.33 1.58 0.21,NS
Abnormal syntax 10 16.67 5 8.33 3 5 3.22 0.065,NS
excessive syntactic constraints 3 5 1 1.67 0 0 1.95 0.14,NS
Lack of semantic association 11 18.33 7 11.67 4 6.67 1.95 0.14,NS
Referential failures 39 65 30 50 9 15 32.15 0.0001,S
Discourse failure 28 46.67 19 31.67 10 16.67 6.63 0.002,S
Excess details 13 21.67 7 11.67 4 6.67 2.83 0.064,NS
Lack of details 21 35 16 26.67 8 13.33 8.39 0.0001,S
Aprosodic speech 9 15 7 11.67 2 3.33 1.98 0.14,NS
Abnormal prosody 17 28.33 10 16.67 6 10 2.79 0.067,NS
Pragmatics disorder 2 3.33 1 1.67 0 0 0.58 0.56,NS
Dysfluency 8 13.33 3 5 1 3.33 1.95 0.14,NS
Dysarthria 0 0 0 0 0 0 …. ….
Poverty of speech 23 38.33 15 25 8 13.33 6.09 0.003,S
Pressure of speech 6 10 4 6.67 1 3.33 0.52 0.59,NS
Neologisms 4 6.67 1 3.33 0 0 1.19 0.30,NS
Paraphasic errors 0 0 0 0 0 0 ….. ……

Table 4: Percentage improvement in individual CLANG domains with treatment over 12 weeks.
CLANG domain %of patients with language dysfunction at baseline %of patients with language dysfunction at 12 weeks Improvement Over 12 weeks
Excess phonetic association 13.33 3.33 10%
Abnormal syntax 16.67 5 11.67%
excessive syntactic constraints 5 0 5%
Lackof semantic association 18.33 6.67 11.67%
Referential failures 65 15 50%
Discourse failure 46.67 16.67 30%
Excess details 21.67 6.67 15%
Lack of details 35 13.33 21.67%
Aprosodic speech 15 3.33 11.67
Abnormal prosody 28.33 10 18.33
Pragmatics disorder 3.33 0 3.33
Dysfluency 13.33 3.33 10
Dysarthria 0 0 0
Poverty of speech 38.33 13.33 25
Pressure of speech 10 3.33 6.67
Neologisms 6.67 0 6.67
Paraphasic errors 0 0 0

Discussion

The present study is relevant as it aims to evaluate the lan- guage dysfunction in patients of psychosis which is a core phe- nomenon of the disorder. Also, few Indian studies have previ- ously reported a dearth of Indian studies in this area [12,13].

The primary objective of our study was to do an in-depth evaluation of the disintegration of components of language in patients with untreated psychosis and the secondary objective was to see the improvement in CLANG domains with treatment in the subjects.

As we chose to take only inpatients into our study, it was possible for us to do a detailed evaluation, which is most of the times not possible in an outpatient setting. Our study is in con- formity with few previous studies which have also attempted to study language dysfunction in psychotic inpatients only [8,16- 18].

We chose to include drug naïve patients only for greater chances of getting undiluted and robust findings for language dysfunction. Our evaluation is supported by most previously conducted most notable studies that also included only drug naïve psychotic patients [11,18].

Comparison of socio-demographic and clinical parameters between previous studies and our study

As per (Table 1) of our results which shows the socio-demo- graphic and clinical profiles of the patients, 73.33% of our pa- tients were in the age group of 21 to 30 years and the mean age was 25.4 years, which is notably the usual age of presen- tation in psychotic patients. On comparing with other studies, we found that the age wise distribution of patients ranged from 23.9 years in the study by to 52.10 years [18,19].

Thus the findings of our study are more in tune with the natural course of age of onset of psychosis.

As far as the gender wise distribution is concerned, males were predominant (68.33%). Few previous studies by Tavano et al. and Murphy et al. have shown the percentage of males to range from 42.3% to 55% [20,21].

68.33% of our patients had studied up to secondary educa- tion. Few previous remarkable studies had their patients with clearly higher mean levels of education compared to our pa- tients [8,14].

All our patients were Hindi-speaking (100%). Most of the previous studies on language dysfunction were done on Eng- lish-speaking people, as reported by Tavano et al. who did their study in Italian-speaking patients for the first time [20].

Regarding the clinical parameters, the age of onset of psy- chosis for most of the patients was found to be between 21 to 30 years (68.33%) and the mean age of onset was 23.9 years. A previous Italian study found the mean age of onset to be 27.40 years in schizophrenic patients [20].

Diagnosis-wise, most of our patients belonged to schizo- phrenia (n=26) (43.33%). A total of seven of them had schizoaf- fective disorder (11.67%), 7 had acute and transient psychotic disorder (11.67%), 3 had persistent delusional disorder (5%), 2 had other non-organic psychotic disorder (3.33%), 6 had severe depressive episodes with psychotic symptoms (10%) and 9 had bipolar affective disorder with psychotic symptoms (15%). On comparison with previous studies, we found that almost all of them also had a spectrum of psychotic patients notable among them being by Andreasen et al. 1979 which had 113 patients (32 manics, 36 depressives and 45 schizophrenic patients), by Chaika (1989) which had 14 schizophrenics and 8 manics, by Chen et al. which had 204 schizophrenic patients [3,14,22].

The mean Duration of Untreated Psychosis (DUP) in our study was 30±0.86 months. It is well known that DUP has a sig- nificant impact on recovery in psychosis However, we could not come across any specific study in relation to DUP and language impairment making any comparison difficult.

Comparison of language dysfunction between previous studies and our study

In our study, language dysfunction was the central focus in 60 patients of untreated psychosis.

It has been long recognized that most disorders of thought can be only be deduced from the speech of the patient but evaluating the language impairment in as many as more than 15 domains is a huge and complex task [9]. A plethora of in- struments have been used by various researchers in evaluating language impairment in patients of psychosis/Schizophrenia previously including Scale for Thought, language and communi- cation by Andreasen and Grove, Thought and language index by [8,23]. Most scales or transcribed interviews could study only two or three domains whereas a comprehensive instrument like CLANG scale has enabled us to objectively measure the 17 dif- ferent domains of language. Notably, Nelli and Crow also used CLANG in their patients [11].

(Table 2) of our results show the language dysfunction among our patients in detail. All the patients had some level of language dysfunction. When mild, moderate and severe lan- guage disturbances in the patients were clubbed together, the most commonly affected language domains were referential failures (65%).

Various other researchers like Hoffman et al have reported problems with reference in language samples of their patients [8,9,24].

The second most common language disturbance in our study was discourse failures (46.67%). This is in keeping with a nota- ble study which said that, in schizophrenia, discourse planning is impaired [24]. Chaika, who studied a single psychotic patient found that her deviant language coincided with her psychotic episodes and otherwise she spoke normally for weeks at a time. The abnormalities that observed were mainly discourse failure and syntactic constraints [25]. These abnormalities have also been noted in our study. In another study, proposed that indi- viduals with schizophrenia often commit errors in which they stray from ‘normal path control’ while speaking and claimed that the disordered discourse of schizophrenics often did not reach its end goal because of ‘grammatical errors [26]. These errors include neologisms which we have also noted in 6.67% of our patients [26].

As regards prosody related language disturbances, abnormal prosody (28.33%) and aprosodic speech (21.67%) were seen in our patients. On comparison with previous studies in this do- main, we found that in previous studies also prosody related problems are seen in language of schizophrenic patients [27- 30].

Abnormal syntax was seen in 16.67% of our patients. This is in keeping with an Italian study which showed that patients with schizophrenia presented with a significant reduction in syntac- tic diversity indices with respect to healthy controls [20]. This is in keeping with a number of other studies as well [5,11,14].

Andreasen commented that the syntax of schizophrenic speech is generally normal, even when the semantics and dis- course organization have completely broken down. Research- ers have demonstrated that schizophrenia is accompanied by a reduction in syntactic complexity and an impairment in syn- tactic comprehension [16,31]. These results were replicated by few researchers who found greater syntactic simplification in patients with negative symptoms than in those with positive symptoms [17,32]. The same research group further found that syntactic complexity diminishes as the chronic patient’s condi- tion deteriorates [32].

None of our patients had dysarthria or paraphasic error.

Lack of semantic association was seen in 18.34% of our pa- tients. Previously few researchers have mentioned semantic abnormalities in their patients [33,34]. Few researchers have found a pattern of relatively preserved syntax combined with more obviously impaired semantics, especially higher-order se- mantics [35].

As reported by various studies, abnormalities in semantic association are commonly proposed to be central to cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenia, with deficits reported on a wide variety of semantic processing tasks [14,36].

Course and progress of improvement of language dysfunc- tion in our study

Most studies in the domains of evaluation of language dys- function have hardly mentioned their course and progress over a period of time which is primarily because most of these stud- ies were single time cross sectional assessments except a few like one by Andreasen and Grove in 1986, which evaluated 100 psychotic patients from four different subgroups over a period of 6 months. However, they failed to comment clearly on the degree of improvement in language dysfunction [8].

As evident from (Table 3) of our study, it can be observed that most of the CLANG scores showed improvement with treat- ment over 12 weeks. The improvement was more between 0 to 4 weeks for excessive phonetic association, abnormal syntax and excess syntactic constraints as well as lack of semantic as- sociation whereas referential failures, discourse failures , excess details and poverty of speech showed more improvement from week 4 to week 12. None of the patients showed any problems with dysarthria or paraphasic errors. Interestingly, neologisms, pragmatics disorder and excess syntactic constraints responded quite effectively and earlier compared to other domains.

As can be seen from (Table 4 and graph 1) of our study, progressive improvement of language abnormalities in our patients over 12 weeks after their natural course of treatment in our short term follow up study showed that referential fail- ures (50%) and discourse failures (30%) followed by poverty of speech (25%), lack of details (21.67%) and abnormal prosody (18.33%). The distribution was found to be significant for ref- erential failures (p value 0.0001), discourse failures (p value 0.002), lack of details (p value 0.0001) and poverty of speech (p value 0.003).

Conclusion

We can proudly say that our study is one of the most thoughtful example of illustrating short term (12 weeks) obser- vation and follow ups for natural course of language dysfunc- tion subjected to psychopharmacological intervention without any comparative studies in literature.

Indeed so far, most of the literature included studies inves- tigating language dimensions in English-speaking people with very few exceptions [20,37].

As reported by a review on studies on language and schizo- phrenia, there are inconsistencies found in and across the stud- ies done in India that need to be addressed [12]. This makes our study all the more relevant in the sea of psychiatric research in the domain of language dysfunction in psychosis.

References

  1. Crow TJ. Schizophrenia as the price that Homo sapiens pays for language: A resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain research reviews. 2000; 31(2-3): 118-29.
  2. Woods WL. Language study in schizophrenia. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1938; 87(3): 290-316.
  3. Andreasen NC. Thought, language, and communication disor- ders: I. Clinical assessment, definition of terms, and evaluation of their reliability. Archives of general Psychiatry. 1979; 36(12): 1315-21.
  4. Andreasen NC. Thought, language, and communication disorders: II. Diagnostic significance. Archives of general Psychiatry. 1979; 36(12): 1325.
  5. DeLisi LE. Speech disorder in schizophrenia: review of the literature and exploration of its relation to the uniquely human capacity for language. Schizophrenia bulletin. 2001; 27(3): 481-96.
  6. Marini A, Spoletini I, Rubino IA, Ciuffa M, Bria P, et al. The language of schizophrenia: An analysis of micro and macrolinguistic abilities and their neuropsychological correlates. Schizophrenia research. 2008; 105(1-3): 144-55.
  7. Chaika E. A linguist looks at schizophrenic language. Brain and language. 1974; 1(3): 257-76.
  8. Andreasen NC, Grove WM. Thought, language, and com-munication in schizophrenia: diagnosis and prognosis. Schizophrenia bulletin. 1986; 12(3): 348-59.
  9. Rochester S. Crazy talk: A study of the discourse of schizophrenic speakers. Springer Science & Business Media. 2013.
  10. Docherty NM, Cohen AS, Nienow TM, Dinzeo TJ, Dangel-maier RE. Stability of formal thought disorder and ref-erential communication disturbances in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003; 112(3): 469.
  11. Ceccherini-Nelli A, Crow TJ. Disintegration of the com-ponents of language as the path to a revision of Bleuler’s and Schneider’s concepts of schizophrenia: Linguistic disturbances compared with first-rank symptoms in acute psychosis. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2003; 182(3): 233-40.
  12. Bhatia TK. Language and thought disorder in multilin-gual schizophrenia. World Englishes. 2019; 38(1-2): 18-29. doi.org/10.1111/weng.12391.
  13. Sultan A, Rizvi T. Language and Schizophrenia: A Review of Studies in India. International Journal of Indian Psy-chȯlogy. 2023; 11(2).
  14. Chen EY, Lam LC, Kan CS, Chan CK, Kwok CL, et al. Language disorganisation in schizophrenia: Val-idation and assessment with a new clinical rating instru-ment. East Asian Archives of Psychiatry. 1996; 6(1): 4.
  15. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. World Health Organization. 1992.
  16. Morice RD, Ingram JC. Language analysis in schizo-phrenia: Diagnostic implications. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 1982; 16(2): 11-21.
  17. Fraser WI, King KM, Thomas P, Kendell RE. The diagnosis of schizophrenia by language analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1986; 148(3): 275-8.
  18. Anand A, Wales RJ, Jackson HJ, Copolov DL. Linguistic impairment in early psychosis. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 1994; 182(9): 488-93.
  19. Taylor MA, Reed R, Berenbaum S. Patterns of speech disorders in schizophrenia and mania. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 1994; 182(6): 319-26.
  20. Tavano A, Sponda S, Fabbro F, Perlini C, Rambaldelli G, et al.. Specific linguistic and pragmatic deficits in Italian patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2008; 102(1-3): 53-62. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.02.008.
  21. Murphy D, Cutting J. Prosodic comprehension and expres-sion in schizophrenia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 1990; 53(9): 727-30. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.53.9.727.
  22. Chaika EO. Understanding Psychotic Speech: Beyond Freud and Chomsky. Charles C Thomas, Publisher. 1990.
  23. Liddle PF, Ngan ET, Caissie SL, Anderson CM, Bates AT, et al. Thought and Language Index: An instrument for assessing thought and language in schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2002; 181(4): 326-30.
  24. Hoffman RE, Stopek S, Andreasen NC. A comparative study of manic vs schizophrenic speech disorganization. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1986; 43(9): 831-8.
  25. Chaika E. A linguist looks at schizophrenic language. Brain and language. 1974; 1(3): 257-76.
  26. Chaika E. A unified explanation for the diverse structural deviations reported for adult schizophrenics with disrupted speech. Journal of Communication Disorders. 1982; 15(3): 167-89.
  27. Rieber RW, Vetter H. The problem of language and thought in schizophrenia: A review. Journal of psycholinguistic Research. 1994; 23: 149-95.
  28. Spoerri TH. Speaking voice of the schizophrenic patient. Archives of general psychiatry. 1966; 14(6): 581-5.
  29. Vetter H. Language behavior & psychopathology. 2024.
  30. Alpert M, Rosen A, Welkowitz J, Sobin C, Borod JC. Vocal acoustic correlates of flat affect in schizophrenia: Similarity to Parkinson’s disease and right hemisphere disease and contrast with depression. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1989; 154(S4): 51-6.
  31. Morice R, McNicol D. Language changes in schizophrenia: A limited replication. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1986; 12(2): 239-51.
  32. Thomas P, King K, Fraser WI, Kendell RE. Linguistic performance in schizophrenia: A comparison of acute and chronic patients. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1990; 156(2): 204-10.
  33. Oh TM, McCarthy RA, McKenna PJ. Is there a schizophasia? A study applying the single case approach to formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Neurocase. 2002; 8(3): 233-44.
  34. Head H. Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. CUP Archive. 1963.
  35. Rodriguez-Ferrera S, McCarthy RA, McKenna PJ. Language in schizophrenia and its relationship to formal thought disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2001; 31(2): 197-205.
  36. Paulsen JS, Romero R, Chan A, Davis AV, Heaton RK, et al. Impairment of the semantic network in schizophrenia. Psychiatry research. 1996; 63(2-3): 109-21.
  37. Sumiyoshi C, Sumiyoshi T, Nohara S, Yamashita I, et al. Disorganization of semantic memory underlies alogia in schizophrenia: An analysis of verbal fluency performance in Japa-nese subjects. Schizophrenia Research. 2005; 74(1): 91-100. ISSN09209964, https://doi.org/10.1016/j. schres.2004.05.011. (https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0920996404001768).